pág. 6410
BEHAVIORAL AND COGNITIVE DIFFERENCES
BETWEEN GIFTED INDIVIDUALS AND THOSE
WITH EXTREMELY HIGH IQ - PEOPLE AT 2SD
AND 3SD
A LOOK AT INCLUSIVE EDUCATION FROM
THE PERSPECTIVE OF A TEACHER IN
THE MATHEMATICS AREA
Mónica García Durán
Investigador Independiente
Darwin Yesid Hernández Hernández
Investigador Independiente
pág. 6411
DOI: https://doi.org/10.37811/cl_rcm.v8i3.11831
Behavioral and Cognitive Differences between Gifted Individuals and Those with
Extremely High IQ - People at 2SD and 3SD
Fabiano de Abreu Agrela Rodrigues
1
deabreu.fabiano@hotmail.com
https://orcid.org/0000-0002-5487-5852
Pós-Phd em Neurociências - Califórnia University
FCE
Aveiro Portugal
Francis Moreira da Silveira
drfrancismsilveira@gmail.com
https://orcid.org/0000-0002-4602-8717
Doutor em Ciências da Saúde - Logos University
International
MG - Brasil
Elodia Avila
elodiaavila@gmail.com
https://orcid.org/0009-0007-0195-0277
Formada em medicina pela USP Especialista em
Cirurgia Plástica
São Paulo - Brasil
Sophia Iris Moreira Utnick Brennan
sophiautnick@hotmail.com
https://orcid.org/0009-0007-6795-3470
Graduada em Justiça Criminal
Estados Unidos
ABSTRACT
Gifted individuals, with IQs 2 standard deviations above the mean, exhibit behaviors more closely associated
with the frontal region of the brain, which may be imbalanced with other areas responsible for different types
of intelligence. This imbalance contributes to a tendency towards maladaptive perfectionism. Despite
demonstrating above-average intelligence, these individuals often show low creativity in terms of innovations
and unconventional thinking. Behaviorally, there is a greater similarity with the autism spectrum, manifesting
through compensatory behaviors. Conversely, individuals with extremely high IQs, 3 standard deviations
above the mean, exhibit more uniform cognitive characteristics, tending to explore a broader range of actions
and showing less rigidity and specificity in their choices. This plurality is associated with greater creativity
based on emotional foundations. They demonstrate less interest in determinism and a reduced need for
competition. Additionally, they possess a more developed instinctive system and a perception of existence that
is less literal and more differentiated. This contrast suggests that while gifted individuals may excel in specific
areas due to their intense focus and specialized skills, those with extremely high IQs benefit from a more
holistic and integrated approach, facilitating creativity and emotional adaptability.
Keywords: giftedness, high IQ, behavior, creativity, perfectionism
1
Autor Principal
Correspondencia: deabreu.fabiano@hotmail.com
pág. 6412
Diferencias conductuales y cognitivas entre personas superdotadas y aquellas
con un coeficiente intelectual extremadamente alto: personas en 2SD y 3SD
RESUMEN
Individuos superdotados, con coeficientes intelectuales 2 desviaciones estándar por encima de la media,
muestran comportamientos más asociados con la región frontal del cerebro, lo cual puede estar desbalanceado
con otras áreas responsables de diferentes tipos de inteligencia. Este desequilibrio contribuye a una tendencia
hacia el perfeccionismo maladaptativo. A pesar de demostrar inteligencia por encima del promedio, estos
individuos a menudo muestran baja creatividad en términos de innovaciones y pensamiento no convencional.
Conductualmente, hay una mayor similitud con el espectro autista, manifestándose a través de
comportamientos compensatorios. Por el contrario, individuos con coeficientes intelectuales extremadamente
altos, 3 desviaciones estándar por encima de la media, muestran características cognitivas más uniformes,
tendiendo a explorar un rango más amplio de acciones y mostrando menos rigidez y especificidad en sus
elecciones. Esta pluralidad se asocia con una mayor creatividad basada en fundamentos emocionales.
Demuestran menos interés en el determinismo y una menor necesidad de competencia. Además, poseen un
sistema instintivo más desarrollado y una percepción de la existencia menos literal y más diferenciada. Este
contraste sugiere que mientras que los individuos superdotados pueden sobresalir en áreas específicas debido
a su enfoque intenso y habilidades especializadas, aquellos con coeficientes intelectuales extremadamente altos
se benefician de un enfoque más holístico e integrado, facilitando la creatividad y la adaptabilidad emocional.
Palabras clave: superdotación, coeficiente intelectual alto, comportamiento, creatividad, perfeccionismo
Artículo recibido 20 mayo 2024
Aceptado para publicación: 21 junio 2024
pág. 6413
INTRODUCTION
The study of cognitive and behavioral differences between gifted individuals and those with extremely high
Intelligence Quotient (IQ) is crucial for understanding the complexities of the human mind. Gifted individuals,
defined as those with an IQ 2 standard deviations (SD) above the average, and those with extremely high IQ
or profound giftedness, 3 SD above the average, display distinctive characteristics that influence their social,
emotional, and intellectual adaptation. Giftedness is not a homogeneous phenomenon; individuals within this
category may exhibit a wide range of specific abilities and challenges, influenced by different brain areas.
Studies suggest that gifted individuals often show an imbalance in the development of different brain areas,
particularly hyperactivity in the frontal region associated with executive functions and impulse control. This
imbalance can contribute to the manifestation of maladaptive perfectionism, where the relentless pursuit of
perfection results in anxiety and frustration. Additionally, these individuals may exhibit limited creativity in
terms of innovations and unconventional thinking, with behaviors often compared to the autism spectrum due
to the compensatory attitudes they exhibit.
Conversely, individuals with extremely high IQ, also categorized as profoundly gifted, present more uniform
and balanced cognitive profiles. They tend to explore a wider range of actions and show less rigidity and
specificity in their behavioral choices. This plurality of interests and approaches is often associated with greater
creativity, grounded on solid emotional foundations. These individuals also demonstrate a lesser need for
competition and a reduced interest in determinism, reflecting a more developed instinctual system and a
perception of existence that is less literal and more differentiated.
This study seeks to analyze and compare the behavioral and cognitive characteristics of gifted individuals (IQ
2 SD above average) and those with extremely high IQ (IQ 3 SD above average). Based on a comprehensive
review of the existing literature, this article aims to identify the particularities of each group, discuss the
implications of these differences for personal and academic development, and provide a foundation for future
research and educational practices.
DEVELOPMENT
To better understand the difference between individuals with 3 SD (Standard Deviation) and 2 SD IQ
(Intelligence Quotient), it is essential to grasp the concept of the normal distribution and how IQ is calculated.
The normal distribution is a curve that represents the frequency of values in a population. The mean is the
central value, and the SD measures the dispersion of values around the mean. In a normal distribution, about
pág. 6414
68% of values fall within 1 SD of the mean, about 95% within 2 SD, and about 99.7% within 3 SD. IQ is
calculated as the ratio of a person's actual intelligence to the average intelligence of the population, multiplied
by 100. The average intelligence is normalized to 100, and the SD is normalized to 15. This means that the
average intelligence is 100 and the SD is 15.
Studies suggest that individuals with high IQ, both gifted (2 standard deviations above the average) and
extremely high IQ or profoundly gifted (3 standard deviations above the average), exhibit significant
distinctions in their cognitive and behavioral profiles. This analysis aims to discuss these differences based on
rigorous scientific evidence. Gifted individuals, defined as those with an IQ 2 standard deviations above the
average, often display a cognitive profile characterized by increased activity in the frontal region of the brain,
associated with executive functions and impulse control. This imbalance between brain areas can lead to
maladaptive perfectionism, where the pursuit of perfection can become counterproductive. Additionally, these
individuals tend to exhibit limited creativity in terms of innovation and outside-the-box thinking. This
phenomenon has been compared to behaviors observed on the autism spectrum, particularly in terms of
compensatory attitudes (Rabiee et al., 2019).
On the other hand, individuals with extremely high IQ (3 standard deviations above the average) demonstrate
more uniform cognitive profiles. Studies indicate that these individuals explore a wider range of actions and
exhibit less rigidity in their behavioral choices. This variability is accompanied by emotionally grounded
creativity, reflecting a greater capacity for divergent thinking and innovative solutions. The reduced need for
competition and a diminished interest in determinism are striking characteristics of this group, suggesting a
more developed instinctual system and a less literal perception of reality (Chiang et al., 2014).
Differences in terms of cognitive processing are also evident. Individuals with high IQ tend to have profiles
characterized by greater capabilities in matrix reasoning and working memory but may show deficiencies in
verbal comprehension and processing speed (Thaler et al., 2010). In contrast, those with extremely high IQ
exhibit a better balance between verbal and performance intelligence, and superior brain efficiency, reflected
in lower metabolic rates during intense mental activities (Deary & Caryl, 1997). Neuropsychological studies
also suggest that IQ profiles can moderately predict behavioral functionality in different contexts. For instance,
in children with traumatic brain injuries, subgroups defined by IQ profiles showed significant variations in
behavioral disorders, with the most compromised groups displaying the greatest disturbances (Thaler et al.,
2010).
pág. 6415
A seminal study compared adolescents with IQs above 130 with their peers with IQs below 130, using a series
of psychological and neuropsychological scales. The results indicated that the adolescents with IQs higher than
130 did not develop more phobias but exhibited greater shyness and fewer friends both in the real and virtual
worlds. Additionally, these adolescents showed greater familial cohesion and did not exhibit a significant
relationship between the socioeconomic status of the parents and the IQ values of the children (Lacour &
Zdanowicz, 2019).
Another study examined the neuropsychological performance of adults in different IQ ranges, finding that
adults with above-average IQs (130-140) significantly outperformed those with average IQs on various
cognitive measures. However, the differences between the IQ 130 and above 140 groups showed that the
precision of IQ scores is not empirically supported, as the 95% confidence intervals could not be reliably
replicated across different intelligence tests (Diaz-Asper, Schretlen, & Pearlson, 2004).
Jaušovec (1998) investigated differences in brain activity between gifted individuals and those with average
IQ during the resolution of tasks. Using electroencephalography (EEG), the study revealed that gifted
individuals exhibited less mental activity (higher alpha power) and less brain complexity (lower Kolmogorov
entropy) in tasks requiring working memory, arithmetic operations, and deductive reasoning, suggesting more
efficient use of brain resources compared to individuals with average IQ. "The conclusion drawn from this
finding is that processing speed is not the main reason for the differences displayed in mental activity. It
appears that the efficiency hypothesis describes much better the outcomes obtained in this study"
(JAUŠOVEC, 1998, p. 264).
Guénolé et al. (2013) investigated the behavioral profiles of gifted children referred to clinics, seeking to
understand if internalizing problems (anxiety, social withdrawal) predominated and if children with higher IQs
(≥145, 3 SD above the average) or with developmental asynchrony (verbal-execution discrepancy) exhibited
more behavioral problems. Contrary to expectations, the study revealed that gifted children referred to clinics
exhibited both internalizing and externalizing problems (aggressiveness, disobedience), with developmental
asynchrony being a risk factor for externalizing and mixed problems. However, no significant differences were
found between children with high IQ (130-144, 2 SD) and very high IQ (≥145, 3 SD) concerning behavioral
problems. "These results suggest that developmental asynchrony is important when examining emotional and
behavioral problems in gifted children" (GNOLÉ et al., 2013, p. 3). The study highlights the importance of
pág. 6416
considering the heterogeneity of development in gifted children, especially asynchrony, when assessing their
emotional and behavioral difficulties.
Brody (2005) discusses a longitudinal study called the Study of Exceptional Talent (SET), which follows
students with exceptional abilities in mathematical and/or verbal reasoning, identified by scoring between 700
and 800 on the math or verbal section of the SAT I before the age of 13. Although the study does not directly
compare individuals with 2 and 3 standard deviations (SD) above the average, it provides relevant information
about the development and characteristics of profoundly talented students. The study discovered that, although
most participants in the SET benefit from rigorous academic programs to complement their school education,
those who achieve the highest scores on general aptitude tests generally require a more individualized
approach. The author emphasizes that these students, with exceptional abilities, may face challenges in finding
learning opportunities that challenge them and match their potential. "Students with such exceptional abilities
may also be at risk of social isolation if their interests and abilities differ greatly from their peers of the same
age" (BRODY, 2005, p. 88).
Runco (1999) presents a longitudinal study with exceptionally talented young people, divided into two groups:
one with an IQ above 150 and another with exceptional talents in mathematics and science. The study does
not focus on a direct comparison between individuals with 2 and 3 standard deviations (SD) above the average.
The study investigated various aspects, including expectations of independence, personality (measured by the
California Psychological Inventory - CPI), and divergent thinking (DT). The results showed that the mothers
of the young people in the high IQ group tended to allow more autonomy compared to the mothers of the
young people talented in mathematics and science. Additionally, flexibility, measured by the CPI, was a
prominent characteristic in both groups, suggesting its importance for creative potential. Regarding divergent
thinking (DT), which is an indicator of creativity, correlations were found with the mothers' expectations of
independence and with some scales of the CPI, such as Communion and Capacity for Status. Interestingly, the
DT scores of the young people did not correlate with their IQs, indicating that creativity and intelligence,
although related, are distinct constructs. The study also explored family dynamics, revealing that the DT of the
young people in the high IQ group correlated with the DT of both parents, while in the math and science group,
the correlation existed only with the mothers.
In interviews with individuals from the Gifted group, with IQs between 130 and 160, we observed that those
with profound giftedness reported receiving more freedom from their parents. The parents justified this
pág. 6417
autonomy based on the maturity and trust demonstrated by their children. As a result, these individuals were
less dedicated to school. In contrast, the gifted group (IQ 130-145) reported greater encouragement and
pressure from parents to achieve good school grades.
The study by Persson (2010) investigates the experiences of intellectually gifted students in an egalitarian and
inclusive educational system in Sweden, where giftedness is not officially recognized. The research involved
287 members of the Swedish Mensa, all with IQs equal to or above the 98th percentile. Using an online
questionnaire, quantitative data were analyzed as dispersions within the group, while qualitative data were
subjected to content analysis. Comparing the experiences of gifted individuals with different IQ levels, the
results suggest that gifted individuals with 2 SD (standard deviations) above the average face significant
challenges in being recognized and supported in the Swedish educational system. Those with 3 SD above the
average face even greater difficulties, often resulting in greater alienation and underutilization of their
intellectual capacities.
Schultz (2018) discusses the characteristics and behaviors of profoundly gifted (PG) students, with IQs 3 or
more standard deviations above the average, in mixed-ability classrooms. The author argues that these students
are rare and often misunderstood in traditional educational settings, due to a lack of knowledge and preparation
by teachers to address their unique needs. The article presents a table with common trends and behaviors in
PG students, such as isolation, complex sense of humor, quest for justice, fast and conceptual learning,
frustration and anxiety in non-challenging environments, high reading ability, existential questioning, and
divergent thinking. These characteristics, although varied and not exhaustive, can help educators and parents
better identify and understand these students. Schultz (2018) emphasizes the importance of differentiating
levels of giftedness so that the specific needs of PG students are recognized and met. The lack of recognition
and adequate support can lead to anxiety, frustration, and even challenging behaviors in these students. The
author suggests that parents and educators seek professional help to develop flexible and individualized
learning plans for these students, and that awareness of the existence and needs of PG students is crucial to
ensure that they reach their full potential.
Shih (2011) investigated the relationship between perfectionism, implicit theories of intelligence, and
academic engagement in Taiwanese eighth-grade students. The study sought to understand how perfectionist
tendencies and beliefs about the nature of intelligence influence academic emotions and students' self-
regulation. The results revealed that adaptive perfectionism, characterized by high personal standards and
pág. 6418
organization, was associated with positive emotions and behavioral self-regulation, related to emotional
intelligence, a term used by some researchers. On the other hand, maladaptive perfectionism, marked by
concern with errors and doubts about one's own actions, was linked to negative emotions and self-sabotage
strategies. Moreover, the research demonstrated that the incremental theory of intelligence, the belief that
intelligence can be developed, predicted positive affect and constructive coping, while the entity theory, which
sees intelligence as fixed, was associated with negative emotions and self-sabotage. The study also identified
distinct profiles of students with different perfectionist tendencies. Adaptive perfectionists exhibited the
healthiest emotions and self-regulation styles, while maladaptive perfectionists demonstrated more
dysfunctional patterns.
Autistic individuals, including those previously diagnosed with Asperger's Syndrome, often face challenges in
social interactions and in understanding and expressing emotions. These challenges can manifest as difficulty
in interpreting social cues, initiating or maintaining conversations, understanding sarcasm, expressing their
own feelings in conventional ways, among others. The intensity and specific characteristics of these challenges
vary greatly from person to person, and some may develop strategies to cope with them. Evidence suggests
that autistic individuals with higher Intelligence Quotients (IQs) may have an easier time developing these
strategies and compensating for some social and emotional difficulties. MacDonald et al. (1989) investigated
the recognition and expression of emotional cues in high-functioning autistic adults compared to normal adults.
Autistic participants exhibited impairment in recognizing emotions in speech and facial expressions, especially
in negative emotions, and their emotional expression was considered atypical. Although no individual test
completely differentiated the groups, a composite score proved effective in identifying socioemotional deficits
in autistic individuals. The authors suggest that this battery of tests may be valuable in family genetic studies
on autism, aiding in the identification of subtle deficits that may be linked to the etiology of autism.
Tucker and Haferstein (1997) explored the sensitivity and emotional intensity in gifted children aged 4 to 6
years, using Dabrowski's theory of overexcitabilities as a theoretical basis. The qualitative research involved
classroom observations, interviews with teachers, and analysis of documents such as individual educational
plans, psychological assessments, and questionnaires completed by parents. The results revealed that all five
children studied exhibited behaviors consistent with Dabrowski's theory, manifesting different forms of
overexcitabilities. All children demonstrated intellectual, imaginative, and emotional overexcitability, while
two of them also exhibited characteristics of psychomotor and sensory overexcitability. Intellectual
pág. 6419
overexcitability manifested through curiosity, formulation of thought-provoking questions, concentration,
problem-solving, and theoretical thinking. Imaginative overexcitability was evident in participation in fantasy
play, animistic and imaginative thinking, daydreaming, and dramatic perception. Emotional overexcitability
was expressed through concern for others, shyness, fear, anxiety, difficulty adjusting to new environments,
and intensity of feelings.
Shaywitz et al. (2001) investigated the heterogeneity within the gifted population, comparing boys with high
IQ (140-154), low IQ (124-139), with learning disabilities, and a normal control group. The results indicated
that boys with high IQ exhibited levels of behavioral problems similar to boys with learning disabilities, while
boys with low IQ demonstrated significantly lower levels of behavioral problems compared to the group with
learning disabilities.
The authors used various measures to assess the participants, including intelligence tests (WISC-R),
performance tests (Woodcock-Johnson Psychoeducational Battery), teacher assessment scales (Abbreviated
Conners Teacher Rating Scale), and questionnaires filled out by parents (Yale Children's Inventory - YCI).
The YCI assesses dimensions of learning disabilities, with an emphasis on attention deficits, and provides
scales that reflect behavioral, attentional, and cognitive characteristics.
Data analysis revealed that the groups differed significantly in terms of socioeconomic status, but not in age
or ethnicity. The average scores on the YCI for the group with learning disabilities were systematically higher
(indicating poorer performance) than the averages for the other three groups, especially on the Habituation,
Language, Attention, and Academic scales. The averages for the normal group were generally in the mid-range
on all measures.
The averages for the low IQ group were the lowest of all the groups (indicating better performance), except
for the Negative Affect and Academic scales. The averages for the high IQ group were generally higher than
the corresponding averages for the normal and low IQ groups, with the Treatability scale showing a particularly
high average. The authors conclude that the gifted population is heterogeneous, with boys with high IQ
exhibiting more behavioral problems than boys with low IQ. This heterogeneity may explain the inconsistent
results of previous studies on gifted children. The authors also suggest that the behavioral characteristics of
boys with high IQ may resemble those found in children with learning disabilities, and that high intelligence
may be a vulnerability factor for social and emotional difficulties.
pág. 6420
"We know very little about highly gifted individuals, partly because their numbers are so small. In the general
population, about 1 in 1000 people are highly gifted. Within the gifted population, this ratio is about 1 in 16.
There is little research on highly gifted adults, although there are some studies on highly gifted children. This
leaflet aims to provide information about high giftedness and to increase awareness and understanding of these
individuals. Our writings and recommendations are based on practical experience, concrete examples, and
literature sources" (IHBV, 2021, p. 1).
"Giftedness is defined as having an IQ within the top 2%, which corresponds to an average score of 100 plus
2 standard deviations. Individuals scoring 3 standard deviations above the mean (IQ of 145 or top 0.1%) are
considered 'highly gifted.' Unfortunately, there is no global consensus on these terms. We use the term
'exceptionally gifted' for IQs of 160 or higher (4 standard deviations above the mean). English also
distinguishes 'profoundly gifted' for IQs above 175. Tests reliably indicating scores above 145-150 are
currently unavailable and cannot precisely determine the 'degree' of giftedness within an IQ number" (IHBV,
2021, p. 2).
"It is likely that the characteristics of highly gifted adults are similar to those of gifted adults in general but
potentially more intense and extreme. Many highly gifted children cannot attend regular schools or skip several
grades. Some even fall completely outside the school system. Finding like-minded individuals is often a
challenge. Even within the group of gifted people, they may still feel 'different'" (IHBV, 2021, p. 3).
"Findings by Hollingworth and Gross show that social connection from childhood is important for highly gifted
people. Finding peers who are intellectually equal is much harder for this group than for the average child.
Brackmann (2020) wrote a book in German about highly gifted adults, emphasizing the importance of social
connection. More attention and study of this subgroup of gifted people are urgently needed for their well-being
and possibly for professional help" (IHBV, 2021, p. 4).
The study "Höchstbegabte Erwachsene: Ihr persönliches Erleben der Begabung und ihre Erfahrung mit
Psychotherapie," conducted by Christina Heil, explores the subjective experiences of highly gifted adults and
their encounters with psychotherapy. The research included 72 participants with an IQ of 145 or higher,
utilizing a qualitative approach with detailed interviews and analysis of personal accounts.
Participants demonstrated highly rapid and complex thinking abilities, with strong logical-analytical skills and
a constant search for patterns. The study also revealed a high moral sensitivity and intense emotional capacities,
frequently showing a deep sense of justice and empathy.
pág. 6421
Many participants reported a persistent feeling of being different from others, resulting in social integration
difficulties and isolation. Additionally, highly gifted individuals often faced underutilization of their abilities
in professional environments, leading to frustration and dissatisfaction.
The participants emphasized the importance of working with therapists who understand the specific needs of
the highly gifted. Collaboration and deep understanding by therapists were seen as crucial for the success of
treatment. Psychotherapy was considered beneficial for many, helping them cope with feelings of isolation,
develop strategies for social integration, and find emotional balance.
The study concludes that there is an urgent need for greater attention and research on highly gifted adults.
Proper understanding of their specific needs and the provision of specialized support are essential for their
well-being. The author stresses the importance of more studies and the creation of more accurate measurement
tools to better serve this population (Heil, 2021).
Analysis of results
Comparative Analysis of Giftedness: Insights from the Genetic Intelligence Project
The Genetic Intelligence Project (GIP), spearheaded by the CPAH - Heráclito Research and Analysis Center,
conducted a comprehensive study examining the differences between individuals with giftedness at two
standard deviations (2SD) and those with profound giftedness at three standard deviations (3SD) above the
mean IQ. The study engaged participants from elite high-IQ societies such as Mensa, Intertel, ISPE, Triple
Nine Society, ePiq, IIS, and ISI, specifically targeting members of the Gifted debate group on Meta, a forum
for individuals with IQs above the 98th percentile.
The findings reveal significant distinctions between the two groups in several areas:
Emotional Well-Being:
Participants with 2SD are perceived to have a lower likelihood of suffering from depression compared to
those with 3SD. Those with 3SD often experience more intense emotional fluctuations, likely due to the
heightened intensity of their characteristic traits.
Creativity
Individuals with 2SD tend to apply their creativity to the knowledge they have acquired, innovating within
familiar domains. In contrast, those with 3SD frequently exhibit creativity in new and unexplored areas,
pushing the boundaries of conventional understanding.
pág. 6422
Social Interactions
The study indicates varied social experiences between the groups, with differences in how they engage and
connect with others both within and outside their intellectual peer group.
These insights contribute to a nuanced understanding of the gifted community, highlighting the need for
tailored support to optimize the well-being and potential of individuals with different levels of giftedness.
The statistical chart reflecting these views is shown below:
Figure 1- Statistical chart
pág. 6423
DISCUSSION
The scientific literature on giftedness, while extensive, still lacks in-depth studies on individuals with
exceptionally high IQs, positioned at 3 or more standard deviations above the average. This gap in knowledge
is partly due to the rarity of this population and the methodological challenges inherent in its research.
However, existing studies reveal an intriguing and complex scenario, highlighting the heterogeneity of the
giftedness experience.
Behavioral and Educational Aspects
Contrary to the expectation that a higher IQ would translate into greater academic success, evidence suggests
that individuals with an IQ 3 SD above the average (3SD) may face more pronounced educational difficulties
than those with an IQ 2 SD (2SD). This discrepancy can be attributed to the greater intensity of behavioral and
emotional traits observed in 3SD individuals, which may lead to mood swings and difficulties adapting to the
traditional school environment. Family dynamics also seem to play a crucial role. Studies indicate that mothers
of 3SD individuals tend to offer greater autonomy and less pressure for academic performance, which may
contribute to lower school engagement. In contrast, 2SD individuals often report greater parental pressure for
academic results, which can boost their performance but also generate anxiety and stress.
Creativity and Expression
Creativity, a trait often associated with giftedness, manifests distinctly in the two groups. 2SD individuals tend
to demonstrate applied creativity, using acquired knowledge to generate innovative solutions in specific areas.
Meanwhile, 3SD individuals exhibit more divergent creativity, exploring original and unconventional ideas,
often related to areas not formally learned.
Emotional and Social Intelligence
Although not measured by traditional IQ tests, emotional and social intelligence appears to be more developed
in 3SD individuals. This enhanced ability to understand and manage emotions, coupled with a more refined
perception of social nuances, can facilitate social interaction and adaptation to different contexts.
Clinical Aspects
While giftedness is not a pathology, the emotional intensity and hypersensitivity characteristic of individuals
with high IQ can increase vulnerability to mental disorders, such as depression and anxiety. However, the
greater emotional intelligence observed in 3SD individuals may act as a protective factor, aiding in emotional
regulation and the development of adaptive coping strategies.
pág. 6424
Giftedness, in its various manifestations, represents both a challenge and an opportunity for society.
Understanding the nuances and heterogeneity of the giftedness experience is crucial for developing effective
educational and clinical interventions that promote the comprehensive development and well-being of these
individuals. Future research should delve deeper into the neurobiological bases of giftedness, investigate the
interaction between genetic and environmental factors in the development of intelligence and creativity, and
examine the impact of different educational and therapeutic approaches on the well-being and success of
individuals with high IQ.
CONCLUSION
The research comparing behavioral and cognitive differences between gifted individuals (2SD above the mean)
and those with extremely high IQ (3SD above the mean) highlights the distinct characteristics and needs of
these groups. The study reveals that while gifted individuals exhibit heightened activity in the frontal brain
regions, contributing to maladaptive perfectionism and limited creativity, individuals with extremely high IQ
display more uniform cognitive profiles and greater emotional and creative adaptability.
Gifted individuals often experience challenges related to their intense focus on perfectionism, which can lead
to anxiety and frustration. Their creativity tends to be less about innovation and more about applying existing
knowledge, often exhibiting behaviors associated with the autism spectrum due to compensatory mechanisms.
These individuals benefit from structured environments that can harness their specific skills and mitigate their
perfectionist tendencies.
Conversely, those with extremely high IQs demonstrate a broader range of cognitive and emotional
capabilities. Their balanced brain activity supports a diverse array of interests and a more profound capacity
for creative and divergent thinking. These individuals are less driven by competition and more by intrinsic
motivation, displaying a nuanced understanding of social dynamics and emotional intelligence. Their holistic
approach to problem-solving and learning allows for greater adaptability and innovation.
The findings underscore the importance of tailored educational and therapeutic interventions for both groups.
For gifted individuals, strategies should focus on managing perfectionism and fostering creativity through
structured challenges. For those with extremely high IQs, the emphasis should be on supporting their broad
intellectual and emotional development, ensuring they are not held back by traditional educational paradigms.
The study calls for more extensive research into the neurobiological and psychological foundations of
giftedness and extremely high IQ. Understanding the unique profiles and needs of these individuals will help
pág. 6425
in developing more effective educational practices and therapeutic approaches. By addressing the specific
challenges and strengths of each group, society can better support their personal and academic growth,
maximizing their potential contributions.
Statement of contributions: Rodrigues, F. A. A. was the idealizer, owner and creator of the concept, wrote
and revised the manuscript. Guided the team in data collection and revised the manuscript.
BIBLIOGRAFHIC REFERENCES
BRODY, Linda E. The study of exceptional talent. High Ability Studies, v. 16, n. 1, p. 87-96, 2005.
CHIANG, H.-M.; TSAI, L.; CHEUNG, Y.; BROWN, A.; LI, H. A meta-analysis of differences in IQ profiles
between individuals with Aspergers disorder and high-functioning autism. Journal of Autism and
Developmental Disorders, 44, p. 1577-1596, 2014.
DEARY, I. J.; CARYL, P. G. Neuroscience and human intelligence differences. Trends in Neurosciences,
1997.
DIAZ-ASPER, C.; SCHRETLEN, D.; PEARLSON, G. How well does IQ predict neuropsychological test
performance in normal adults? Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 10, p. 82-90,
2004.
GUÉNOLÉ, Fabian et al. Behavioral profiles of clinically referred children with intellectual giftedness.
BioMed Research International, v. 2013, 2013.
IHBV. High giftedness. [S.l.], Jan. 2021. Available at: https://ihbv.nl/wp-content/uploads/202101-High-
giftedness.pdf. Accessed on: June 10, 2024.
JAUŠOVEC, Norbert. Are gifted individuals less chaotic thinkers?. Personality and Individual Differences, v.
25, n. 2, p. 253-267, 1998.
HEIL, Christina. Höchstbegabte Erwachsene: Ihr persönliches Erleben der Begabung und ihre Erfahrung mit
Psychotherapie. 2021.
LACOUR, A.-G.; ZDANOWICZ, N. IQ over 130 and phobia: correlation, consequences and other
psychopathologies. Psychiatria Danubina, 31 Suppl 3, p. 386-389, 2019.
MACDONALD, Hope et al. Recognition and expression of emotional cues by autistic and normal adults.
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, v. 30, n. 6, p. 865-877, 1989.
PERSSON, Roland S. Experiences of Intellectually Gifted Students in an Egalitarian and Inclusive Educational
System: A Survey Study. Journal for the Education of the Gifted, v. 33, n. 4, p. 536569, 2010.
pág. 6426
RABIEE, A.; SAMADI, S. A.; VASAGHI-GHARAMALEKI, B.; HOSSEINI, S.; SEYEDIN, S.; KEYHANI,
M.; MAHMOODIZADEH, A. The cognitive profile of people with high-functioning autism spectrum
disorders. Behavioral Sciences, 9, 2019.
RUNCO, Mark A. A longitudinal study of exceptional giftedness and creativity. Creativity Research Journal,
v. 12, n. 2, p. 161-164, 1999.
SCHULTZ, R. A. Recognizing the Outliers: Behaviors and Tendencies of the Profoundly Gifted Learner in
Mixed-Ability Classrooms. Roeper Review, v. 40, n. 3, p. 191196, 2018.
SHAYWITZ, Sally E. et al. Heterogeneity within the gifted: Higher IQ boys exhibit behaviors resembling
boys with learning disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, v. 34, n. 1, p. 26-39, 2001.
SHIH, S.-S. Perfectionism, Implicit Theories of Intelligence, and Taiwanese Eighth-Grade Students
Academic Engagement. The Journal of Educational Research, v. 104, n. 2, p. 131142, 2011.
THALER, N. S.; BELLO, D. T.; RANDALL, C.; GOLDSTEIN, G.; MAYFIELD, J.; ALLEN, D. IQ profiles
are associated with differences in behavioral functioning following pediatric traumatic brain injury.
Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 25(8), p. 781-790, 2010.
TUCKER, Brooke; HAFERSTEIN, Norma Lu. Psychological intensities in young gifted children. Gifted Child
Quarterly, v. 41, n. 1, p. 66-75, 1997.